What were our
ancestors like before there was anything like religion? Were they like bands of
chimpanzees? What, if anything, did they talk about, aside from food and predators
and the mating game? Do the burial practices of Neanderthals show that they
must have had fully articulate language?
Could an ape (without
language) concoct the counterintuitive combination of a walking tree or an
invisible banana? Why don't other species have art? Why do we human beings so
consistently focus our fantasies on our ancestors? Does impromptu hypnosis
work as effectively when the hypnotist is not the parent? How well have
nonliterate cultures preserved their rituals and creeds over the generations?
How did healing rituals arise? Does there have to be someone to prime the pump?
(What is the role of charismatic innovators in the origin of religious
groups?)
For how long
could folk religion be carried along by our ancestors before reflection began
to transform it? How and why did folk religions metamorphose into organized
religions?
Why do people
join groups? Is the robustness of a religion like the robustness of an ant
colony or a corporation? Is religion the product of blind evolutionary
instinct or rational choice? Or is there some other possibility? Are Stark and
Finke right about the principal reason for the precipitous decline after
Vatican II in Catholics seeking a vocation in the church?
Of all the people who
believe in belief in God, what percentage (roughly) also actually believe in
God? At first it looks as if we could simply give people a questionnaire with a
multiple choice question on it:
I believe in God: __
Yes __ No __ I don't know
Or should the
question be:
God exists:
__ Yes __ No __ I
don't know
Would it make any difference how we framed the
questions?
You will notice that
hardly any of these questions deal even indirectly with either brains or genes.
Why not? Because having religious convictions is not very much like having
either epileptic seizures or blue eyes. We can already be quite sure there isn't
going to be a "God gene," or even a "spirituality" gene,
and there isn't going to be a Catholicism center in the brain of Catholics, or
even a "religious experience" center. Yes, certainly, whenever you
think of Jesus some parts of your brain are going to be more active than others,
but whenever you think of anything this is going to be true. Before we start
coloring in your particular brain-maps for thinking about jesting and Jet Skis
and jewels (and Jews), we should note the evidence that suggests that such hot
spots are both mobile and multiple, heavily dependent on context-and of course
not arrayed in alphabetical order across the cortex! In fact, the likelihood
that the places that light up today when you think about Jesus are the same
places that will light up next week when you think about Jesus is not very
high. It is still possible that we will find dedicated neural mechanisms for
some aspects of religious experience and conviction, but the early forays into
such research have not been persuasive)
Until we develop
better general theories of cognitive architecture for the representation of
content in the brain, using neuro-imaging to study religious beliefs is almost
as hapless as using a voltmeter to study a chess-playing computer. In due
course, we should be able to relate everything we discover by other means to
what is going on among the billions of neurons in our brains, but the more
fruitful paths emphasize the methods of psychology and the other social
sciences.
Humans who developed
a spiritual sense thrived and bequeathed that trait to their offspring. Those
who didn't risked dying out in chaos and killing. The evolutionary equation is
a simple but powerful one.
The idea that lurks
in this bold passage is that religion is "good for you" because it
was endorsed by evolution. This is just the sort of simpleminded Darwinism that
rightly gives the subtle scholars and theorists of religion the heebie-jeebies.
Actually, as we have seen, it isn't that simple, and there are more powerful
evolutionary "equations." The hypothesis that there is a
(genetically) heritable "spiritual sense" that boosts human genetic
fitness is one of the less likely and less interesting of the evolutionary
possibilities. In place of a single spiritual sense we have considered a
convergence of several different overactive dispositions, sensitivities, and
other coopted adaptations that have nothing to do with God or religion. We did
consider one of the relatively straightforward genetic possibilities, a gene
for heightened hypnotizability. This might have provided
major health benefits in earlier times, and would be one way of taking the
"God gene" hypothesis seriously. Or we could put it together with
William Tames's old speculation that there are two
kinds of people, those who require "acute" religion and those whose
needs are "chronic" and milder. We can try to discover if there
really are substantial organic differences between those who are highly
religious and those whose enthusiasm for religion is moderate to nonexistent.
Suppose we struck
paydirt and found just such a pattern. What would be the implications-if
any-for policy? We could consider the parallel with the genetic differences
that help to account for some Asians' and some Native Americans' difficulty
with alcohol. As with variation in lactose tolerance, there is genetically
transmitted variation in the ability to metabolize alcohol, due to variation
in the presence of enzymes, mainly alcohol dehydrogenase and aldehyde
dehydrogenase. (Troy Duster, "Race and Reification in Science."
Science, vol. 307, 2005, pp. 105-51.)
Needless to say,
since, through no fault of their own, alcohol is poisonous to people with these
genes-or it turns them into alcoholics-they are well advised to forgo alcohol.
A different parallel is with the genetically transmitted distaste for broccoli
and cauliflower and cilantro that many people discover in themselves; they have
no difficulty metabolizing these foods, but find them unpalatable, because of
identifiable differences in the many genes that code for olfactory receptors.
They don't have to be advised to avoid these foods. Might there be either
"spiritual experience intolerance" or "spiritual-experience
distaste"? There might be. There might be psychological features with
genetic bases that are made manifest in different reactions by people to
religious stimuli (however we find it useful to classify these). William James
offers informal observations that give us some reason to suspect this. Some
people seem impervious to religious ritual and all other manifestations of
religion, whereas others-like me-are deeply moved by the ceremonies, the music,
and the art-but utterly unpersuaded by the doctrines. It may be that still
others hunger for these stimuli, and feel a deep need to integrate them into
their lives, but would be well advised to steer clear of them, since they can't
"metabolize" them the way other people can. (They become manic and
out of control, or depressed, or hysterical, or confused, or addicted.)
These are hypotheses
that are definitely worth formulating in detail and testing if we can identify
patterns of individual variation, whether or not they are genetic (they might
be culturally transmitted, after all). To take a fanciful example, it could
turn out that people whose native language was Finnish (whatever their genetic
heritage) were well advised to moderate their intake of religion.
A "spiritual
sense" (whatever that is) might prove to be a genetic adaptation in the
simplest sense, but more specific hypotheses about patterns in human tendencies
to respond to religion are apt to be more plausible, more readily tested, and more
likely to prove useful in disentangling some of the vexing policy questions
that we have to face. For instance, it would be particularly useful to know
more about how secular beliefs differ from religious beliefs,
"belief" is a misnomer here; we might better call them religious
convictions to mark the difference). How do religious convictions differ from
secular beliefs in the manner of their acquisition, persistence, and
extinction, and in the roles they play in people's motivation and behavior?
There has been a substantial the many genes that code for olfactory receptors. They
don't have to be advised to avoid these foods. Might there be either
"spiritual experience intolerance" or "spiritual-experience
distaste"? There might be. There might be psychological features with
genetic bases that are made manifest in different reactions by people to
religious stimuli (however we find it useful to classify these). William James
offers informal observations that give us some reason to suspect this. Some
people seem impervious to religious ritual and all other manifestations of
religion, whereas others-like me-are deeply moved by the ceremonies, the music,
and the art-but utterly unpersuaded by the doctrines. It may be that still
others hunger for these stimuli, and feel a deep need to integrate them into
their lives, but would be well advised to steer clear of them, since they can't
"metabolize" them the way other people can. (They become manic and
out of control, or depressed, or hysterical, or confused, or addicted.)
We regularly see the
highlights of the latest results in the media, but the theoretical
underpinnings and enabling assumptions of the survey methodologies are in need
of careful analysis. Alan Wolfe, for one, thinks that the surveys are
unreliable: "The results are inconsistent and puzzling, depending, as is
often the case with such research, on the wording of the questions in surveys
or the samples chosen for analysis." But is Wolfe right? This should not
just be a matter of personal opinion. We need to find out.
According to ARIS
(American Religious Identification Survey) in 2001, the three categories with
the largest gain in membership since the previous survey of 1990 were
evangelical/born-again (42 percent), nondenominational (37 percent), and no
religion (23 percent). These data support the view that evangelicalism is
growing in the U.S.A., but they also support the view that secularism is on the
rise. We are apparently becoming polarized, as many informal observers have recently
maintained. Why? Is it because, as supply-siders such as Stark and Finke think,
only the most costly religions can compete with no religion at all in the
marketplace for our time and resources? Or is it that the more we learn about
nature, the more science strikes many people as leaving something out,
something that only an antiscience perspective can
seem to supply? Or is there some other explanation?
Before we jump in to
explain the data, we should ask how sure we are of the assumptions used in
gathering them. Just how reliable are the data, and how were they gathered?
(Telephone inquiry, in the case of ARIS, not written questionnaire.) What
checks were used to avoid biasing context? What other questions were people
asked? How long did it take to conduct the interview?
And then there are
offbeat questions that might have answers that mattered:
What had happened in
the news on the day the poll was conducted? Did the interviewer have an accent?
And so on.7 Large-scale surveys are expensive to conduct, and nobody spends
thousands of dollars gathering data using a casually designed "instrument"
(questionnaire). Much research has been devoted to identifying the sources of
bias and artifact in survey research. When should you use a simple yes/no
question (and don't forget to include the important "I don't know"
option), and when should you use a five-point Likert scale (such as the
familiar strongly agree, tend to agree, uncertain, tend to disagree, strongly
disagree)? When ARIS did its survey in 1990, the first question was: "What
is your religion?" In 2001, the question was amended: "What is your
religion, if any?" How much of the increase in Non-denominational and No
religion was due to the change in wording? Why was the "if any"
phrase added?
In the course of writing
How We Believe: Science, Skepticism and the Search for God (2nd ed., 2003),
Michael Shermer, the director of the Skeptic Society, conducted an ambitious
survey of religious convictions. The results are fascinating, in part because
they differ so strikingly from the results found in other, similar surveys.
Most recent surveys find approximately 90 percent of Americans believe in
God-and not just an "essence" God, but a God who answers prayers. In
Shermer's survey, only 64 percent said they believed in God-and 25 percent said
they disbelieved in God (p. 79). That's a huge discrepancy, and it is not due
to any simple sampling error (such as sending the questionnaires to known
skeptics!). Shermer speculates that education is the key. His survey asked people
to respond in their own words to "an open-ended essay question"
explaining why they believed in God:
As it turns out, the
people who completed our survey were significantly more educated than the
average American, and higher education is associated with lower religiosity.
According to the V.S. Census Bureau for 1998, one-quarter of Americans over
twenty-five years old have completed their bachelor's degree, whereas in our
sample the corresponding rate was almost twothirds.
(It is hard to say why this was the case, but one possibility is that educated
people are more likely to complete a moderately complicated survey.) [P.79]
But as David Polk
pointed out, once self-selection is acknowledged as a serious factor, we should
ask the further question: who would take time to fill out such a
questionnaire? Probably only those with the strongest beliefs. People who just
don't think religion is important are unlikely to fill out a questionnaire that
involves composing answers to questions. Only one out of ten of the people who
received the mailed-out survey returned it, a relatively low rate of return, so
we can't draw any interesting conclusions from his 64 percent figure, as he
acknowledges.
Children
A research topic of
particular urgency, but also particular ethical and political sensitivity, is
the effect of religious upbringing and education on young children. There is an
ocean of research, some good, some bad, on early-childhood development, on language
learning and nutrition and parental behavior and the effect of peers and just
about every other imaginable variable that can be measured in the first dozen
years of a person's life, but almost all of this-so far as I can
determine-carefully sidesteps religion, which is still largely terra incognita.
Sometimes there are very good indeed, unimpeachable-ethical reasons for this.
All the carefully erected and protected barriers to injurious medical research
with human subjects apply with equal force to any research we might imagine
conducting on variations in religious upbringing. We aren't going to do placebo
studies in which group A memorizes one catechism while group B memorizes a
different catechism and group C memorizes nonsense syllables. We aren't going
to do cross-fostering studies in which babies of Islamic parents are switched
with babies of Catholic parents. These are clearly off limits, and should
remain so. But what are the limits? The question is important, because, as we
try to design indirect and noninvasive ways of getting at the evidence we seek,
we will confront the sort of trade-offs that regularly confront researchers
looking for medical cures. Perfectly risk-free research on these topics is
probably impossible. What counts as informed consent, and how much risk may
even those who consent be permitted to tolerate? And whose consent? The
parents' or the children's?
All these policy
questions lie unexamined in the shadows cast by the first spell, the one that
says that religion is out of bounds, period. We should not pretend that this
is benign neglect on our part, since we know full well that under the
protective umbrellas of personal privacy and religious freedom there are
widespread practices in which parents subject their own children to treatments
that would send any researcher, clinical or otherwise, to jail. What are the
rights of parents in such circumstances, and "where do we draw the
line"? This is a political question that can be settled not by discovering
"the answer" but by working out an answer that is acceptable to as
many informed people as possible.
It will not please
everybody, any more than our current laws and practices regarding the
consumption of alcoholic beverages please everybody. Prohibition was tried, and
by general consensus-far from unanimous-it was determined to be a failure. The
current understanding is quite stable; we are unlikely to go back to Prohibition
anytime soon. But there are still laws forbidding the sale of alcoholic
beverages to minors (with age varying by country). And there are plenty of gray
areas: what should we do if we find parents giving alcohol to their children?
At the ball game, the parents may get in trouble, but what about in the privacy
of their own homes? And there is a difference between a glass of champagne at
big sister's wedding, and a six-pack of beer every evening while trying to do
homework. When do the authorities have not just the right but the obligation to
step in and prevent abuse? Tough questions, and they don't get easier when the
topic is religion, not alcohol. In the case of alcohol, our political wisdom is
importantly informed by what we have learned about the short-term and long-term
effects of imbibing it, but in the case of religion we're still flying blind.
We'd be aghast to be
told of a Leninist child or a neo-conservative child or a Hayekian monetarist
child. So isn't it a kind of child abuse to speak of a Catholic child or a
Protestant child? Especially in Northern Ireland and Glasgow where such labels,
handed down over generations, have divided neighbourhoods
for centuries and can even amount to a death warrant? Or imagine if we
identified children from birth as young smokers or drinking children because
their parents smoked or drank.
In mammals and
birds who must care for their offspring the instinct to protect one's young
from all outside interference is universal and extremely potent; we will risk
our lives unhesitatingly-unthinkingly-to fend off threats, real or imagined.
It's like a reflex. And in this case, we can "feel in our bones" that
parents do have the right to raise their children the way they see fit. Never
make the mistake of wandering in between a mother bear and her cub, and nothing
should come between parents and their children. That's the core of
"family values." At the same time, we do have to admit that parents
don't literally own their children (the way slave owners once owned slaves),
but are, rather, their stewards or guardians and ought to be held accountable
by outsiders for their guardianship, which does imply that outsiders have a
right to interfere-which sets off that adrenaline alarm again. When we find
that what we feel in our bones is hard to defend in the court of reason, we
get defensive and testy, and start looking around for something to hide behind.
How about a sacred and (hence) unquestionable bond? Ah, that's the ticket!
There is an obvious
(but seldom discussed) tension between the supposedly sacred principles invoked
at this point. On the one hand, many declare, there is the sacred and
inviolable right to life: every unborn child has a right to life, and no
prospective parent has the right to terminate a pregnancy (except maybe if the
mother's life is itself in jeopardy). On the other hand, many of the same people
declare that, once born, the child loses its right not to be indoctrinated or
brainwashed or otherwise psychologically abused by those parents, who have the
right to raise the child with any upbringing they choose, short of physical
torture. Let us spread the value of freedom throughout the world-but not to
children, apparently. No child has a right to freedom from indoctrination.
Shouldn't we change that? What, and let outsiders have a say in how I raise my
kids?
While we wrestle with
the questions about the Andaman Islanders, we can see that we are laying the
political foundations for similar questions about religious upbringing in
general. We shouldn't assume, while worrying over the likely effects, that the
seductions of Wester culture will automatically swamp all the fragile treasures
of other cultures. It is worth noting that many Muslim women, raised under
conditions that many non-Muslim women would consider intolerable, when given
informed opportunities to abandon their veils and many of their other
traditions, choose instead to maintain them.
Maybe people
everywhere can be trusted, and hence allowed to make their own informed
choices. Informed choice! What an amazing and revolutionary idea! Maybe people
should be trusted to make choices, not necessarily the choices we would
recommend to them, but the choices that have the best chance of satisfying
their considered goals. But what do we teach them until they are informed
enough and mature enough to decide for themselves? We teach them about all the
world's religions, in a matter-of-fact, historically and biologically informed
way, the same way we teach them about geography and history and arithmetic.
Let's get more education about religion into our schools, not less. We should
teach our children creeds and customs, prohibitions and rituals, texts and
music, and when we cover the history of religion, we should include both the
positive-the role of the churches in the civil-rights movement of the 1960s,
the flourishing of science and the arts in early Islam, and the role of the
Black Muslims in bringing hope, honor, and self-respect to the otherwise
shattered lives of many inmates in our prisons, for instance-and the
negative-the Inquisition, anti-Semitism over the ages, the role of the
Catholic Church in spreading AIDS in Africa through its opposition to condoms.
No religion should be favored, and none ignored. And as we discover more and
more about the biological and psychological bases of religious practices and
attitudes, these discoveries should be added to the curriculum, the same way we
update our education about science, health, and current events. This should all
be part of the mandated curriculum for both public schools and home-schooling.
Here's a proposal,
then: as long as parents don't teach their children anything that is likely to
close their minds
1. through fear
or hatred or
2. by disabling
them from inquiry (by denying them an education, for instance, or keeping them
entirely isolated from the world)
then they may teach
their children whatever religious doctrines they like. It's just an idea, and
perhaps there are better ones to consider, but it should appeal to freedom
lovers everywhere: the idea of insisting that the devout of all faiths should
face the challenge of making sure their creed is worthy enough, attractive and
plausible and meaningful enough, to withstand the temptations of its
competitors. If you have to hoodwink-or blindfold-your children to ensure that
they confirm their faith when they are adults, your faith ought to go extinct.
Toxic memes?
Consider the selfobservation of Raja Shedadeh,
writing about the grip of modern Palestine: "Most of your energy is spent
extending feelers to detect public perception of your actions, because your
survival is contingent on remaining on good terms with your society."
When we can
share similar observations about the problems in our own society, we will be
on a good path to mutual understanding. Palestinian society, if Shehadeh is
right, is beset with a virulent case of the "punish those who won't
punish" idea, for which there are models that predict other properties we
should look for. It may be that this particular feature would foil
well-intentioned projects that would work in societies that lack it. In
particular, we mustn't assume that policies that are benign in our own culture
will not be malignant in others. As Jessica Stern puts it:
I have come to see
terrorism as a kind of virus, which spreads as a result of risk factors at
various levels: global, interstate, national, and personal. But identifying
these factors precisely is difficult. The same variables (political,
religious, social, or all of the above) that seem to have caused one person to
become a terrorist might cause another to become a saint. [Terror in the Name
of God, 2003, p.283]
As communications
technology makes it harder and harder for leaders to shield their people from
outside information, and as the economic realities of the twenty-first century
make it clearer and clearer that education is the most important investment any
parent can make in a child, the floodgates will open all over the world, with
tumultuous effects. All the flotsam and jetsam of popular culture, all the
trash and scum that accumulates in the corners of a free society, will
inundate these relatively pristine regions along with the treasures of modern
education, equal rights for women, better health care, workers' rights,
democratic ideals, and openness to the cultures of others. As the experience in
the former Soviet Union shows only too clearly, the worst features of
capitalism and high tech are among the most robust replicators in this
population explosion of memes, and there will be plenty of grounds for
xenophobia, Luddism, and the tempting "hygiene" of backward-looking
fundamentalism. At the same time, we shouldn't rush to be apologetic about
American pop culture. It has its excesses, but in many instances it is not the
excesses that offend so much as the egalitarianism and tolerance. The hatred
of this potent American export is often driven by racism-because of the strong
Afro-American presence in American pop culture-and sexism-because of the status
of women we celebrate and our (relatively) benign treatment of homosexuality.
Researchers don't
have to be believers to be understanders, and we had better hope I was right,
since we want our researchers to understand Islamic terrorism from the inside
without having to become Muslims-and certainly not terrorists-in the process.
But we also won't understand Islamic terrorism unless we can see how it is like
and unlike other brands of terrorism, including Hindu and Christian terrorism,
ecoterrorism, and antiglobalist terrorism, to round
up the usual suspects. And we won't understand Islamic and Hindu and Christian
terrorism without understanding the dynamics of the transitions that lead from
benign sect to cult to the sort of disastrous phenomenon we witnessed in
Jonestown, Guyana, in Waco, Texas, and in the Aum Shinrikyo cult in Japan.
One of the most
tempting hypotheses is that these particularly toxic mutations tend to arise
when charismatic leaders miscalculate in their attempts to be memetic
engineers, unleashing memetic adaptations that they find, like the Sorcerer's
Apprentice, they can no longer control. They then become somewhat desperate,
and keep reinventing the same bad wheels to carry them over their excesses. The
anthropologist Harvey Whitehouse (1995) offers an account of the debacle that
overtook the leaders of Pomio Kivung,
the new religion in Papua New Guinea, that suggests (to me) that something like
runaway sexual selection took over. The leaders responded to the pressure from
the people-Prove that you mean itt-with ever-inflated
versions of the claims and promises that had brought them to power, leading
inevitably to a crash. It's reminiscent of the accelerated burst of creativity
you see in pathological liars when they can sense that their exposure is
imminent. Once you've talked the people into killing all the pigs in
anticipation of the great Period of the Companies, you have nowhere to go but
down. Or out: It's them-the infidels-who are the cause of all our misery!
There are so many
complexities, so many variables-can we ever hope to make predictions that we
can act on? Yes, in fact, we can. Here is just one: in every place where
terrorism has blossomed, those it has attracted are almost all young men who
have learned enough about the world to see that their futures look otherwise
bleak and uninspiring (like the futures of those who were preyed upon by Marjoe
Gortner).
What seems to be most
appealing about militant religious groups-whatever combination of reasons an
individual may cite for joining-is the way life is simplified. Good and evil
are brought out in stark relief. Life is transformed through action. Martyrdom-the
supreme act of heroism and worship-provides the ultimate escape from life's
dilemmas, especially for individuals who feel deeply alienated and confused,
humiliated or desperate.
Where are we going to
find an overabundance of such young men in the very near future? In many
countries, but especially in China, where the draconian one-child-per-family
measures that have slowed the population explosion so dramatically (and turned
China into a blooming economic force of unsettling magnitude) have had the side
effect of creating a massive imbalance between male and female children.
Everybody wanted to have a son (a superannuated meme that had evolved to thrive
in an earlier economic environment), so daughters have been aborted (or killed
at birth) in huge numbers, so now there are not going to be anywhere near
enough wives to go around. What are all those young men going to do with
themselves? We have a few years to figure out benign channels into which their
hormone-soaked energies can be directed.
Instead of trying to
destroy the madrassahs that close the minds of thousands of young Muslim boys,
we should create alternative schools-for Muslim boys and girls-that will better
serve their real and pressing needs, and let these schools compete openly with
the madrassahs for clientele. And how can we hope to compete with the promise
of salvation and the glories of martyrdom? We could lie, and make promises of
our own that could never be fulfilled in this life or anywhere else, or we
could try something more honest: we could suggest to them that the claims of
any religion should, of course, be taken with a grain of salt. We could start
to change the climate of opinion that holds religion to be above discussion,
above criticism, above challenge. False advertising is false advertising, and
if we start holding religious organizations accountable for their claims-not
by taking them to court but just by pointing out, often and in a
matter-of-fact tone of voice, that of course these claims are ludicrous-perhaps
we can slowly get the culture of credulity to evaporate. We have mastered the
technology for creating doubt through the mass media ("Are you sure your
breath is sweet?" "Are you getting enough iron?" "What has
your insurance company done for you lately?"), and now we can think about
applying it, gently but firmly, to topics that have heretofore been off limits.
Let the honest religions thrive because their members are getting what they
want, as informed choosers.
But we can also start
campaigns to adjust specific aspects of the landscape in which this competition
takes place. A bottomless pit in that landscape that strikes me as particularly
deserving of paving over is the tradition of "holy soil." Here is
Yoel Lerner, an Israeli and a former terrorist, quoted by Stern:
"There are six hundred thirteen commandments in the Torah. The temple
service accounts for about two hundred and forty of these. For nearly two
millennia, since the destruction of the Temple, the Jewish people, contrary to
their wishes, have been unable to maintain the temple service. They've been
unable to comply with those commandments. The Temple constituted a kind of
telephone line to God," Lerner summarizes. "That link has been destroyed.
We want to rebuild it."
Nonsense, say I. Here
is an imaginary case: Suppose it turned out that Liberty Island (formerly
Bedloe's Island, on which the Statue of Liberty stands) was once a burial
ground of the Mohawks-say the Matinecock Tribe of
nearby Long Island. And suppose the Mohawks came forward with the claim that it
should be restored to pristine purity (no gambling casinos, but also no Statue
of Liberty, just one big holy cemetery). Nonsense. And shame on any Mohawks
who had the chutzpah to rile up their fellow braves on the issue. This would be
ancient history-a lot less ancient than the history of the Temple-and it
should be allowed to recede gracefully into the past.
We don't let
religions declare that their holy traditions require that left-handed people be
enslaved, or that people who live in Norway should be killed. We similarly
cannot let religions declare that "infidels" who have been innocently
living on their "holy" turf for generations have no right to live
there. There is also, of course, culpable hypocrisy in the policy of
deliberately building new settlements in order to create just such
"innocent" dwellers and foreclose the claims of the previous dwellers
on that land. This is a practice that goes back centuries; the Spaniards who
conquered most of the Western Hemisphere often took care to build their
Christian churches on the destroyed foundations of the temples of the indigenous
people. Out of sight, out of mind. Neither side of these disputes is above
criticism. If we could just devalue the whole tradition of holy soil, and its
occupation, we could address the residual injustices with clearer heads.
Perhaps you disagree
with me about this. Fine. Let's discuss it calmly and openly, with no untmmpable appeals to the sacred, which have no place in
such a discussion. If we should continue to honor claims about holy soil, it
will be because, all things considered, this is the course of action that is
just, and life-enabling, and a better path to peace than any other we can find.
Any policy that cannot pass that test doesn't deserve respect.
Such open discussions
are underwritten by the security of a free society, and if they are to continue
unmolested, we must be vigilant in protecting the institutions and principles
of democracy from subversion. Remember Marxism? It used to be a sour sort of
fun to tease Marxists about the contradictions in some of their pet ideas. The
revolution of the proletariat was inevitable, good Marxists believed, but if
so, why were they so eager to enlist us in their cause? If it was going to
happen anyway, it was going to happen with or without our help. But of course
the inevitability that Marxists believe in is one that depends on the growth
of the movement and all its political action. There were Marxists working very
hard to bring about the revolution, and it was comforting to them to believe
that their success was guaranteed in the long run. And some of them, the only
ones that were really dangerous, believed so firmly in the rightness of their
cause that they believed it was permissible to lie and deceive in order to
further it. They even taught this to their children, from infancy. These are
the "red-diaper babies," children of hardline members of the
Communist Party of for example America, and some of them can still be found
infecting the atmosphere of political action in left-wing circles, to the
extreme frustration and annoyance of honest socialists and others on the left.
Today we have a
similar phenomenon brewing on the religious right: the inevitability of the End
Days, or the Rapture, the coming Armageddon that will separate the blessed from
the damned in the final Day of Judgment. Cults and prophets proclaiming the imminent
end of the world have been with us for several millennia, and it has been
another sour sort of fun to ridicule them the morning after, when they
discover that their calculations were a little off But, just as with the
Marxists, there are some among them who are honor claims about holy soil, it
will be because, all things considered, this is the course of action that is
just, and life-enabling, and a better path to peace than any other we can find.
Any policy that cannot pass that test doesn't deserve respect.
Such open discussions
are underwritten by the security of a free society, and if they are to continue
unmolested, we must be vigilant in protecting the institutions and principles
of democracy from subversion. Remember Marxism? It used to be a sour sort of
fun to tease Marxists about the contradictions in some of their pet ideas. The
revolution of the proletariat was inevitable, good Marxists believed, but if
so, why were they so eager to enlist us in their cause? If it was going to
happen anyway, it was going to happen with or without our help. But of course
the inevitability that Marxists believe in is one that depends on the growth
of the movement and all its political action. There were Marxists working very
hard to bring about the revolution, and it was comforting to them to believe
that their success was guaranteed in the long run. And some of them, the only
ones that were really dangerous, believed so firmly in the rightness of their
cause that they believed it was permissible to lie and deceive in order to
further it. They even taught this to their children, from infancy. These are
the "red-diaper babies," children of hardline members of the
Communist Party of America, and some of them can still be found infecting the
atmosphere of political action in left-wing circles, to the extreme
frustration and annoyance of honest socialists and others on the left.
Today we have a
similar phenomenon brewing on the religious right: the inevitability of the End
Days, or the Rapture, the coming Armageddon that will separate the blessed from
the damned in the final Day of Judgment. Cults and prophets proclaiming the imminent
end of the world have been with us for several millennia, and it has been
another sour sort of fun to ridicule them the morning after, when they
discover that their calculations were a little off But, just as with the
Marxists, there are some among them who are working hard to "hasten the
inevitable," not merely anticipating the End Days with joy in their
hearts, but taking political action to bring about the conditions they think
are the prerequisites for that occasion. And these people are not funny at all.
They are dangerous, for the same reason that red-diaper babies are dangerous:
they put their allegiance to their creed ahead of their commitment to democracy,
to peace, to (earthly) justice-and to truth. If push comes to shove, some of
them are prepared to lie and even to kill, to do whatever it takes to help
bring what they consider celestial justice to those they consider the sinners.
Are they a lunatic fringe? They are certainly dangerously out of touch with
reality, but it is hard to know how many they are. Are their numbers growing?
Apparently. Are they attempting to gain positions of power and influence in
the governments of the world? Apparently. Should we know all about this
phenomenon? We certainly should. A poll in Newsweek (May 24, 2004)
claimed that 55 percent of Americans think that the faithful will be
taken up to heaven in the Rapture and 17 percent believe the world will end in
their lifetimes. If this is even close to being accurate, it suggests that End
Timers in the first decade of the twenty first century outnumber the Marxists
of the I930’s through the I950’s by a wide margin.
Hundreds of Web sites
purport to deal with this phenomenon, but I am not in a position to endorse any
of them as accurate, so I will not list any. This in itself is worrisome, and
constitutes an excellent reason to conduct an objective investigation of the
whole End Times movement, and particularly the possible presence of fanatical
adherents in positions of power in the government and the military. What can we
do about this? I suggest that the political leaders who are in the best
position to call for a full exposure of this disturbing trend are those whose
credentials could hardly be impugned by those who are fearful of atheists or
brights: the eleven senators and congressmen who are members of the
"Family" (or the "Fellowship Foundation"), a secretive
Christian organization that has been influential in Washington, D.C., for
decades: Senators Charles Grassley (R., Iowa), Pete Domenici (R., N.Mex.),
John Ensign (R., Nev.), James Inhofe (R., Okla.), Bill Nelson (D., Fla.),
Conrad Bums (R., Mont.), and Representatives Jim DeMint (R., S.e.), Frank Wolf (R., Va.), Joseph Pitts (R., Pa.), Zach
Wamp (R., Tenn.), and Bart Stupak (D., Mich.). Its current leader, Douglas Coe,
is described by Time magazine (February 7, 2005, p. 41) as "the Stealth
Persuader."
But, in the end,
would it not be better to educate the people of the world, so that they can
make truly informed choices about their lives? Ignorance is nothing shameful;
imposing ignorance is shameful. Most people are not to blame for their own
ignorance, but if they willfully pass it on, they are to blame. One might think
this is so obvious that it hardly needs proposing, but in many quarters there
is substantial resistance to it. People are afraid of being more ignorant than
their children especially, apparently, their daughters. We are going to have
to persuade them that there are few pleasures more honorable and joyful than
being instructed by your own children. It will be fascinating to see what
institutions and projects our children will devise, building on the foundations
earlier generations have built and preserved for them, to carry us all safely
into the future.
For my earlier article about the involvement of Christianity with WWI
click:
For updates
click homepage here